TY - JOUR
T1 - Does one hour of bright or short-wavelength filtered tablet screenlight have a meaningful effect on adolescents' pre-bedtime alertness, sleep, and daytime functioning?
AU - Heath, Melanie
AU - Sutherland, Cate
AU - Bartel, Kate
AU - Gradisar, Michael
AU - Williamson, Paul
AU - Lovato, Nicole
AU - Micic, Gorica
PY - 2014/5
Y1 - 2014/5
N2 - Electronic media use is prevalent among adolescent populations, as is the frequency of sleeplessness. One mechanism proposed for technology affecting adolescents' sleep is the alerting effects from bright screens. Two explanations are provided. First, screens emit significant amounts of short-wavelength light (i.e. blue), which produces acute alertness and alters sleep timing. Second, later chronotypes are hypothesised to be hypersensitive to evening light. This study analysed the pre-sleep alertness (GO/NOGO task speed, accuracy; subjective sleepiness), sleep (sleep diary, polysomnography), and morning functioning of 16 healthy adolescents (M=17.4±1.9yrs, 56% f) who used a bright tablet screen (80lux), dim screen (1lux) and a filtered short-wavelength screen (f.lux; 50lux) for 1hr before their usual bedtime in a within-subjects protocol. Chronotype was analysed as a continuous between-subjects factor; however, no significant interactions occurred. Significant effects occurred between bright and dim screens for GO/NOGO speed and accuracy. However, the magnitude of these differences was small (e.g. GO/NOGO speed=23ms, accuracy=13%), suggesting minimal clinical significance. No significant effects were found for sleep onset latency, slow-rolling eye movements, or the number of SWS and REM minutes in the first two sleep cycles. Future independent studies are needed to test short (1hr) vs longer (>2hrs) screen usage to provide evidence for safe-to-harmful levels of screenlight exposure before adolescents' usual bedtime.
AB - Electronic media use is prevalent among adolescent populations, as is the frequency of sleeplessness. One mechanism proposed for technology affecting adolescents' sleep is the alerting effects from bright screens. Two explanations are provided. First, screens emit significant amounts of short-wavelength light (i.e. blue), which produces acute alertness and alters sleep timing. Second, later chronotypes are hypothesised to be hypersensitive to evening light. This study analysed the pre-sleep alertness (GO/NOGO task speed, accuracy; subjective sleepiness), sleep (sleep diary, polysomnography), and morning functioning of 16 healthy adolescents (M=17.4±1.9yrs, 56% f) who used a bright tablet screen (80lux), dim screen (1lux) and a filtered short-wavelength screen (f.lux; 50lux) for 1hr before their usual bedtime in a within-subjects protocol. Chronotype was analysed as a continuous between-subjects factor; however, no significant interactions occurred. Significant effects occurred between bright and dim screens for GO/NOGO speed and accuracy. However, the magnitude of these differences was small (e.g. GO/NOGO speed=23ms, accuracy=13%), suggesting minimal clinical significance. No significant effects were found for sleep onset latency, slow-rolling eye movements, or the number of SWS and REM minutes in the first two sleep cycles. Future independent studies are needed to test short (1hr) vs longer (>2hrs) screen usage to provide evidence for safe-to-harmful levels of screenlight exposure before adolescents' usual bedtime.
KW - Adolescents
KW - Alertness
KW - Chronotype
KW - Light
KW - REM sleep
KW - Sleep
KW - Technology
UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?scp=84897498123&partnerID=8YFLogxK
U2 - 10.3109/07420528.2013.872121
DO - 10.3109/07420528.2013.872121
M3 - Article
SN - 0742-0528
VL - 31
SP - 496
EP - 505
JO - Chronobiology International
JF - Chronobiology International
IS - 4
ER -